HANDWRITING TRAINING: DOES IT IMPROVE WORKING MEMORY IN ELEMENTARY SCHOOL STUDENTS?

Main Article Content

Ida Bagus Aditya Vyad Tirtha
Jap Tji Beng
Tasya Mulia Salsabila
Sri Tiatri

Abstract

The development of information and digital technology is currently growing very rapidly. One proof of the very rapid progress of information and digital technology is the increase in the amount of information that can be obtained from various sources. The ability needed to process new information to help understand learning is working memory. Working memory is short-term memory in the brain for storing and manipulating information so as to help the learning process and understanding a person's behavior. Working memory skills are important to be trained from an early age. There is a need for learning about working memory which can be developed in elementary schools. Previous research shows that working memory can be improved through handwriting training. However, research on the effect of handwriting training on working memory in Indonesia is still very limited. This research aims to determine the effect of handwriting training on working memory in elementary school students. The method used is quasi-experimental. Participants in this research were 4th grade elementary school students. This research uses an experimental group and a control group. The experimental group was given handwriting training for 10 days. The experimental group and control group were given a pre-test before the experiment and a post-test after the experiment to measure the differences between the two groups. The results of this research can support the level of awareness regarding the importance of handwriting training for elementary school students.

Article Details

Section
Articles

References

Atkinson, R.C. and Shiffrin, R.M., 1968. Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. Psychology of learning and motivation, 2, 89-195.

Bastos, A. M., Miller, E. K., & Lundqvist, M. (2018). Perspective. Cellpress, 463–475 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2018.09.023

Bawden, D. and Robinson, L., 2009. The dark side of information: overload, anxiety and other paradoxes and pathologies. Journal of information science, 35(2), 180-191.

Beaudoin, C. E. (2008). Explaining the relationship between internet use and interpersonal trust:

Taking into account motivation and information overload. Journal of Computer-Mediated

Communication, 13, 550–568. doi:10.1111/j.1083-6101.2008.00410.x

Benselin, J. C., & Ragsdell, G. (2015). Information overload: The differences that age makes. Journal of Librarianship and Information Science. doi:10.1177/0961000614566341

Cobos, A. V., & Contreras, R. S. (2018). American Futurology as a Sociological problem. International Journal of Business and Social Science, 9(11). https://doi.org/10.30845/ijbss.v9n11p3

Cook, T. D. (2015). Quasi-Experimental Design. Wiley Encyclopedia of Management, 1–2. doi:10.1002/9781118785317.weom110227

Eppler, M. J. (2015). Information quality and information overload: The promises and perils of the information age. In L. Cantoni & J. A. Danowski (Eds.), Communication and technology (pp. 215–232). Berlin: de Gruyter Mouton

Eppler, M. J., & Mengis, J. (2004). The concept of information overload: A review of literature from organization science, accounting, marketing, MIS, and related disciplines. The Information Society, 20, 325–344. doi:10.1080/01972240490507974

Gwizdka, J., 2013. Effects of working memory capacity on users' search effort. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Multimedia, Interaction, Design and Innovation ACM, 11.

Gwizdka, J., 2017. I Can and So I Search More: Effects Of Memory Span On Search Behavior. In Proceedings of the 2017 Conference on Conference Human Information Interaction and Retrieval ACM, 341-344.

Höchtl, J., Parycek, P., & Schöllhammer, R. (2016). Big data in the policy cycle: Policy decision making in the digital era. Journal of Organizational Computing and Electronic Commerce, 26(1–2), 147–169. https://doi.org/10.1080/10919392.2015.1125187

Jensen, A. R., & Osborne, R. T. (1979). Forward and backward digit span interaction with race and IQ: A longitudinal developmental comparison. Berkeley: University of California. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 173 384).

Kemendikbud Ristek (2023). Bahan Bacaan Literasi 2016--2019. Badan Bahasa Kemdikbud. https://badanbahasa.kemdikbud.go.id/produk-detail/751/bahan-bacaan-literasi-2016--2019

Lichtsteiner, S. H., Wicki, W., Falmann, P. (2018). Impact of handwriting training on fluency, spelling and text quality among third graders. Reading and Writing, 31, 1295-1318. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-018-9825-x

Putra, A. S., Tiatri, S., Sutikno, N. (2017). Peningkatan kapasitas working memory melalui permainan congklak pada siswa sekolah dasar. Jurnal Psikologi. DOI:10.22146/jpsi.21984

Nordenson, B. (2008). Overload! Columbia Journalism Review, 47(4), 30–42.

Thorson, E., Reeves, B., & Schleuder, J. (1985). Message complexity and attention to television. Communication Research, 12, 427–454. doi:10.1177/009365085012004001

Wechsler, D. (2008). Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Fourth Edition. San Antonio, TX: Pearson.

Wicki, W., Lichtsteiner, S. H., Geiger, A.S., Muller, M. (2014). Handwriting fluency in children: Impact and Correlates. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 73(2). https://doi.org/10.1024/1421-0185/a000127